鶹ֱapp

“We are very grateful that these three individuals came forward and allowed us to test them so we could document their syndrome and try to understand the brain mechanisms behind it,” said Professor Brian Levine (photo by _annamo via Flickr)

Living in the third person: this memory glitch affects healthy, high-functioning people

“They cannot re-experience the past with a vivid sense of personal reliving. It's as if their past was experienced in the third person,” says researcher

Imagine living a healthy, normal life without the ability to remember personal events from your past. You have heard about them from family and friends, but you can't see or imagine yourself in any of them.

Cognitive scientists from the University of Toronto had a rare opportunity to examine three middle-aged adults (two from the U.S., the other from the U.K.) who live their lives in the "third person" because of a condition known as lifelong severely deficient autobiographical memory (SDAM).

The intriguing findings are posted online in the journal , ahead of the print edition.

“Many of us can relate to the idea that people have different abilities when remembering events. What is unique about these individuals is that they have no personal recollection,” said Professor Brian Levine. The senior author on the paper, Levine is a professor in the Faculty of Medicine’s division of neurology and Institute of Medical Science as well as senior scientist at Baycrest Health Sciences’ Rotman Research Institute.

“Even though they can learn and recall information normally and hold down professional careers, they cannot re-experience the past with a vivid sense of personal reliving. It's as if their past was experienced in the third person.”

People with SDAM have no history of amnesia, brain injury, birth complications, seizures, stroke, neurological disease or psychological disorder to explain their memory syndrome. They are highly educated and functioning normally in their occupational and social roles. While scientists are unsure how rare the condition is, the Canadian study represents the first opportunity to examine the memory syndrome in healthy individuals in a lab setting with brain imaging and reliable and valid memory testing protocols.

The research team discovered reduced vividness, visualization and recollection of both personal events and laboratory-presented material in the three adults compared to control subjects. Using different methods of brain imaging – structural and functional magnetic resonance imaging and electroencephalography (EEG) – the team found evidence of brain differences in the SDAM cases compared to control subjects. The researchers also examined the size of the hippocampus, which plays a critical role in memory, and found a subtle volume reduction in the right hippocampus in the SDAM cases.

When presented with events from their own life, the SDAM cases had reduced activation in midline brain regions that play a prominent role in autobiographical memory processes, such as mental time travel. When their memory for pictures was tested, the EEG signals associated with conscious recollection in healthy adults were greatly reduced in the SDAM cases, even though their recognition of the pictures was normal. This suggested that the SDAM cases can learn, but without a normal recollective experience.

“The SDAM cases had very little access to autobiographical details from their remote past, including visualization of those experiences and their emotional state,” said  鶹ֱapp alumna Dr. Daniela Palombo, the study's lead author and currently a post-doctoral researcher at VA Boston Healthcare System and Boston University School of Medicine.

“We also found significant activation reductions in core regions known to play a role in autobiographical memory, which may provide some of the explanation for their inability to mentally project the self through time and their reduced visualization abilities, compared to the matched controls.”

Yet the findings also suggested how it is that the SDAM cases can still function normally in day-to-day life. They rely on intact non-recollective memory, such as rehearsed factual knowledge about themselves or others.

“There is a lot that we can accomplish with these skills, which are also part of normal memory function,” said Levine. The SDAM syndrome may involve a lifetime of compensation using these abilities.

Palombo cautioned that further studies are required to tease out other possible neurological factors and health issues that may play a role in SDAM.

“We are very grateful that these three individuals came forward and allowed us to test them so we could document their syndrome and try to understand the brain mechanisms behind it,” added Levine.

SDAM is the extreme opposite of another syndrome known as highly superior autobiographical memory (HSAM), which refers to individuals with the uncanny ability to recall a vast amount of episodic autobiographical memory details.

Kelly Connelly is senior media officer at Baycrest Health Sciences’ Rotman Research Institute, a partner of the University of Toronto.

The Bulletin Brief logo

Subscribe to The Bulletin Brief